早期语言学习的影响

Fernando 卢比奥, 2022 University of Utah

早期朗简报

突出了

  • Just like in first language learning, age can influence how well we learn a second language.
  • 课堂语言学习, the amount of time and the kind of classroom activities used may predict success more than age.
  • Starting earlier and learning a second language for a longer period of time is most likely to lead to proficiency.
  • Because older children and adults have experience in learning languages and learning in general, these groups can sometimes make great progress even in short periods of time.

We often hear that children are amazing at acquiring their first language (L1); by the time they reach school age, they are using complex grammar and demonstrating a wide vocabulary. 更令人印象深刻的是, these young learners might show up at school with two languages in their toolbox or add a second language (L2) with far less effort than older children and certainly their adult counterparts. The differences in learning between children and adults are frequently used to promote the general assumption that, 说到语言学习, 越年轻越好. As this piece shows, the facts are more complicated. 此外, some research suggests that there are benefits to learning languages beyond developing language proficiency.

文档2 x
可打印版本 下载PDF

将此摘要保存起来供您使用.

“When there are constraints on the available instructional time, a later start to L2 learning seems to be advisable since older learners can tap into their advanced cognitive abilities and make faster progress.”

费尔南多·卢比奥,犹他大学

Age and language learning in naturalistic settings

One issue with research on language acquisition is that a lot of it centers on acquisition in naturalistic settings (like the home or the community) rather through formal learning (like the classroom). There is also research that suggests that age-related factors make it impossible to acquire a first language after a specific period of time. This assumption is called the Critical Period Hypothesis (CPH).

Some researchers proposed that the CPH applies to L2 learning just as it does to L1 learning- this means it might be impossible to learn a second language after a specific age as well! One important focus of such research examines the influence of age as an ultimate predictor of language. 研究表明, although older children and adults may make faster progress initially in L2 learning, younger children always have an advantage in terms of ultimate attainment. This means that native-like competence and, 特别是, the lack of a non-native accent is only possible (albeit not guaranteed) with an early start (see Granena & 长,2013年).

Learning in the world language classroom

Although there is a lot of research that shows that children learn languages quickly and effectively in naturalistic settings, there’s less to support that classroom learning of language is as helpful to young children’s acquisition as naturalistic learning. 事实上,一些研究人员(例如.g., Patkowski, 2003) argue that the CPH applies only to naturalistic learning contexts in which language acquisition occurs mostly by exposure.

In many parts of the world including the United States language programs are often described as “drip-feed” instruction, meaning that students receive a small number of hours of instruction per week over a period of several years. This approach produces small proficiency gains (see, for example, Davin, Rempart & Hammerand, 2014), and starting earlier does not seem to make a difference (Muñoz & 位咨询专家,2019). In a large study that examined the impact of children’s ages on classroom language learning, 巴塞罗那年龄因子(BAF)项目, 这一发现得到了证实. The BAF project examined students learning English at ages 8, 11, 14岁及18岁以上, and the researchers looked at students’ progress after 200, 416和726小时的教学. The results of the BAF project showed that older starters made faster progress initially but that the younger learners caught up with the older learners in listening and speaking over time; the overall number of hours of exposure was more important than the age at which the children started (Muñoz, 2006). Other authors have similarly suggested that a key factor in L2 learning is the quantity and quality of exposure to the target language (Flege, 2018).

In addition to developing language proficiency, language instruction has been associated with other academic benefits. 例如, 阿姆斯特朗和罗杰斯(1997), comparing elementary students over one semester who had three Spanish lessons per week versus those who had not learned Spanish, found that the Spanish students had significantly better scores on 数学 and language. 类似的, Taylor and Lafayette (2010) compared students who had 30 minutes of world language instruction per day with those who had not, 跨三年级,4, 和5, 在路易斯安那州. They examined the student test scores on the Iowa Test of Basic Skills and the Louisiana Educational Assessment Program for the 21st Century. 作者证明了, for each additional year of world language instruction and controlling for other factors, the world language students significantly outperformed their non-world language counterparts at every level.

曝光的长度和类型

Why do young children and adults acquire languages in such different ways? It may be that different learning mechanisms are more appropriate for different age groups. 例如, DeKeyser (2003) has suggested that children learn implicitly or in naturalistic settings but they need massive amounts of input for this to happen. 另一方面, older learners have cognitive skills from having learned in formal settings, and they can learn in classroom settings. Long (2013) has suggested that the optimal situation for instructed L2 learning would involve starting learners as early as possible in programs that offer the type of “quasi-naturalistic” exposure that is typical of language immersion settings. 在这种情况下, young learners could still take advantage of their implicit learning mechanisms and, because of their length of exposure to the language, they would be able to reach high levels of proficiency.

Recently, dual language immersion (DLI) programs have become popular in the U.S. 在犹他州, the state follows a 50/50 DLI model which means that students have equal amounts of exposure to English and another language beginning in first grade and continuing throughout elementary school. 这种教学模式, which means the language is taught through school subjects like 数学 and social studies, is closer to the naturalistic setting described above. Data collected over several years in the Utah DLI programs show that children attain high levels of proficiency (Watzinger-Tharp, 卢比奥 & Tharp, 2018; Watzinger-Tharp, Tharp & 卢比奥,2021). Starting so early and with such a focus on input means that their proficiency continues to develop at a steady pace beyond the early years. In this learning context, an early start results in very positive outcomes.

外卖

Learning language has positive impacts both in the language classroom and beyond. Findings about the impact of age in L2 acquisition can be crucially relevant for classroom learning since they may inform parents and administrators regarding the best time to introduce language learning or the most appropriate duration of a language program. Evidence from research shows that an early start may be advantageous when children are exposed to language in situations that approximate what occurs in a naturalistic setting: implicit learning and ample amounts of input over an extended period. 另一方面, when there are constraints on the available instructional time, a later start to L2 learning seems to be advisable since older learners can tap into their advanced cognitive abilities and make faster progress.

引文和参考文献